Color  

From The Art and Popular Culture Encyclopedia

(Difference between revisions)
Jump to: navigation, search
Revision as of 13:35, 19 October 2016
Jahsonic (Talk | contribs)

← Previous diff
Revision as of 20:06, 9 March 2019
Jahsonic (Talk | contribs)

Next diff →
Line 1: Line 1:
[[Image:Ultramarinepigment.jpg|thumb|left|200px|''[[Blue]]'' of the ultramarine variant, similar to the [[International Klein Blue]] used by [[Yves Klein]]]] [[Image:Ultramarinepigment.jpg|thumb|left|200px|''[[Blue]]'' of the ultramarine variant, similar to the [[International Klein Blue]] used by [[Yves Klein]]]]
 +{| class="toccolours" style="float: left; margin-left: 1em; margin-right: 2em; font-size: 85%; background:#c6dbf7; color:black; width:30em; max-width: 40%;" cellspacing="5"
 +| style="text-align: left;" |
 +"There are certain effects of [[color|colour]] that give all men [[pleasure]], and others which [[jar]], almost like a musical [[discord]]. A more general development of this sensibility would make possible a new [[abstract art]], an art that should deal with colours as music does with sound." --''[[The Sense of Beauty]]'' (1896)
 +|}
 +[[Image:Black Square by Malevich.jpg|thumb|right|200px|''[[Black Square]]'' (1915) by [[Kazimir Malevich]]]]
[[Image:Sunday Afternoon on the Island of La Grande Jatte (1884-1886) - Seurat.jpg|thumb|right|200px|''[[A Sunday Afternoon on the Island of La Grande Jatte]]'' ([[1884]]-[[1886]]) - [[Georges Seurat]]]] [[Image:Sunday Afternoon on the Island of La Grande Jatte (1884-1886) - Seurat.jpg|thumb|right|200px|''[[A Sunday Afternoon on the Island of La Grande Jatte]]'' ([[1884]]-[[1886]]) - [[Georges Seurat]]]]
[[Image:Combat de nègres dans un tunnel.jpg|thumb|right|200px| [[Image:Combat de nègres dans un tunnel.jpg|thumb|right|200px|
Line 11: Line 16:
The science of color is sometimes called '''''chromatics''''', '''''[[colorimetry]]''''', or simply '''''color science'''''. It includes the perception of color by the [[human eye]] and brain, the origin of color in materials, [[color theory]] in [[art]], and the [[physics]] of [[electromagnetic radiation]] in the visible range (that is, what we commonly refer to simply as ''[[light]]''). The science of color is sometimes called '''''chromatics''''', '''''[[colorimetry]]''''', or simply '''''color science'''''. It includes the perception of color by the [[human eye]] and brain, the origin of color in materials, [[color theory]] in [[art]], and the [[physics]] of [[electromagnetic radiation]] in the visible range (that is, what we commonly refer to simply as ''[[light]]'').
- 
-== Physics of color == 
- 
-[[File:Rendered Spectrum.png|thumb|400px|Continuous optical spectrum rendered into the [[sRGB]] color space.]] 
- 
-{| class="wikitable" style="float:right; width:400px; margin:1em 0 1em 1em; clear:right;" 
-|+ '''The colors of the visible light spectrum'''<ref>{{cite book | title = Fundamentals of Atmospheric Radiation: An Introduction with 400 Problems | author = Craig F. Bohren | publisher = Wiley-VCH | year = 2006 | isbn = 3-527-40503-8 | url = https://books.google.com/?id=1oDOWr_yueIC&pg=PA214&lpg=PA214&dq=indigo+spectra+blue+violet+date:1990-2007 }}</ref> 
-|- 
-!style="text-align: left" colspan="2" |color 
-! abbr="wavelength" | Wavelength<br>interval 
-! abbr="frequency" | Frequency<br>interval 
-|- 
-! style="background:#f00;"| 
-!style="text-align: left"|[[Red]] 
-| ~ 700–635&nbsp;nm 
-| ~ 430–480&nbsp;THz 
-|- 
-! style="background:#ff8000"| 
-!style="text-align: left"|[[orange (colour)|Orange]] 
-| ~ 635–590&nbsp;nm 
-| ~ 480–510&nbsp;THz 
-|- 
-! style="background:#ff0"| 
-!style="text-align: left"|[[Yellow]] 
-| ~ 590–560&nbsp;nm 
-| ~ 510–540&nbsp;THz 
-|- 
-! style="background:#0f0"| 
-!style="text-align: left"|[[Green]] 
-| ~ 560–520&nbsp;nm 
-| ~ 540–580&nbsp;THz 
-|- 
-! style="background:#0ff"| 
-!style="text-align: left"|[[Cyan]] 
-| ~ 520–490&nbsp;nm 
-| ~ 580–610&nbsp;THz 
-|- 
-! style="background:#00f"| 
-! style="text-align: left"|[[Blue]] 
-| ~ 490–450&nbsp;nm 
-| ~ 610–670&nbsp;THz 
-|- 
-! style="background:#8000ff" | 
-!style="text-align: left"|[[violet (color)|Violet]] 
-| ~ 450–400&nbsp;nm 
-| ~ 670–750&nbsp;THz 
-|} 
- 
-{| class="wikitable" style="float:right; width:400px; margin:hem 0 1em 1em; clear:right;" 
-|+ '''Color, wavelength, frequency and energy of light''' 
-|- 
-!style="text-align: left"|Color 
-!<math>\lambda \,\!</math> 
-(nm) 
-!<math>\nu \,\!</math> 
-(THz) 
-!<math>\nu_b \,\!</math> 
-(μm<sup>&minus;1</sup>) 
-![[Photon energy|<math>E \,\!</math>]] 
-(eV) 
-!<math>E \,\!</math> 
-(kJ mol<sup>&minus;1</sup>) 
-|- style="text-align:right;" 
-!style="text-align: left"|[[Infrared]] 
-| >1000 
-| <300 
-| <1.00 
-| <1.24 
-| <120 
-|- style="text-align:right;" 
-!style="text-align: left"|Red 
-| 700 
-| 428 
-| 1.43 
-| 1.77 
-| 171 
-|- style="text-align:right;" 
-!style="text-align: left"|Orange 
-| 620 
-| 484 
-| 1.61 
-| 2.00 
-| 193 
-|- style="text-align:right;" 
-!style="text-align: left"|Yellow 
-| 580 
-| 517 
-| 1.72 
-| 2.14 
-| 206 
-|- style="text-align:right;" 
-!style="text-align: left"|Green 
-| 530 
-| 566 
-| 1.89 
-| 2.34 
-| 226 
-|- style="text-align:right;" 
-!style="text-align: left"|Blue 
-| 470 
-| 638 
-| 2.13 
-| 2.64 
-| 254 
-|- style="text-align:right;" 
-!style="text-align: left"|Violet 
-| 420 
-| 714 
-| 2.38 
-| 2.95 
-| 285 
-|- style="text-align:right;" 
-!style="text-align: left"|Near [[ultraviolet]] 
-| 300 
-| 1000 
-| 3.33 
-| 4.15 
-| 400 
-|- style="text-align:right;" 
-!style="text-align: left"|Far ultraviolet 
-| <200 
-| >1500 
-| >5.00 
-| >6.20 
-| >598 
-|} 
- 
-<!-- no empty line here --> 
- 
-[[Electromagnetic radiation]] is characterized by its [[wavelength]] (or [[frequency]]) and its [[Luminous intensity|intensity]]. When the wavelength is within the visible spectrum (the range of wavelengths humans can perceive, approximately from 390&nbsp;[[nanometre|nm]] to 700&nbsp;nm), it is known as "visible light". 
- 
-Most light sources emit light at many different wavelengths; a source's ''spectrum'' is a distribution giving its intensity at each wavelength. Although the spectrum of light arriving at the eye from a given direction determines the color [[Wikt:sensation|sensation]] in that direction, there are many more possible spectral combinations than color sensations. In fact, one may formally define a color as a class of spectra that give rise to the same color sensation, although such classes would vary widely among different species, and to a lesser extent among individuals within the same species. In each such class the members are called ''[[Metamerism (color)|metamers]]'' of the color in question. 
- 
-=== Spectral colors === 
- 
-The familiar colors of the [[rainbow]] in the [[Optical spectrum|spectrum]] – named using the [[Latin]] word for ''appearance'' or ''apparition'' by [[Isaac Newton]] in 1671 – include all those colors that can be produced by [[visible light]] of a single wavelength only, the [[spectral color|''pure spectral'' or ''monochromatic'' colors]]. The table at right shows approximate frequencies (in [[hertz|terahertz]]) and wavelengths (in [[nanometre|nanometers]]) for various pure spectral colors. The wavelengths listed are as measured in air or [[vacuum]] (see [[refractive index]]). 
- 
-The color table should not be interpreted as a definitive list – the pure spectral colors form a continuous spectrum, and how it is divided into distinct colors [[language|linguistically]] is a matter of culture and historical contingency (although people everywhere have been shown to ''perceive'' colors in the same way<ref>[[Brent Berlin|Berlin, B.]] and [[Paul Kay|Kay, P.]], ''[[Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution]]'', Berkeley: [[University of California Press]], 1969.</ref>). A common list identifies six main bands: red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet. Newton's conception included a seventh color, [[indigo]], between blue and violet. It is possible that what Newton referred to as blue is nearer to what today we call [[cyan]], and that indigo was simply the dark blue of the [[indigo dye]] that was being imported at the time.<ref>{{cite book|last=Waldman|first=Gary|title=Introduction to light : the physics of light, vision, and color|year=2002|publisher=Dover Publications|location=Mineola|isbn=978-0-486-42118-6|page=193|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=PbsoAXWbnr4C&pg=PA193&dq=Newton+color+Indigo&hl=en&ei=kw3QTemzJsea8QOC_anbDQ&sa=X&oi=book_result&ct=result&resnum=7&ved=0CH0Q6AEwBg#v=onepage&q=Newton%20color%20Indigo&f=false|edition=Dover}}</ref> 
- 
-The ''intensity'' of a spectral color, relative to the context in which it is viewed, may alter its perception considerably; for example, a low-intensity orange-yellow is [[brown]], and a low-intensity yellow-green is olive-green. 
- 
-=== Color of objects === 
- 
-The color of an object depends on both the physics of the object in its environment and the characteristics of the perceiving eye and brain. Physically, objects can be said to have the color of the light leaving their surfaces, which normally depends on the spectrum of the incident illumination and the reflectance properties of the surface, as well as potentially on the angles of illumination and viewing. Some objects not only reflect light, but also transmit light or emit light themselves, which also contribute to the color. A viewer's perception of the object's color depends not only on the spectrum of the light leaving its surface, but also on a host of contextual cues, so that color differences between objects can be discerned mostly independent of the lighting spectrum, viewing angle, etc. This effect is known as [[color constancy]]. 
- 
-[[File:Optical grey squares orange brown.svg|right|thumb|250px|The upper disk and the lower disk have exactly the same objective color, and are in identical gray surroundings; based on context differences, humans perceive the squares as having different reflectances, and may interpret the colors as different color categories; see [[checker shadow illusion]].]] 
- 
-Some generalizations of the physics can be drawn, neglecting perceptual effects for now: 
- 
-* Light arriving at an [[Opacity (optics)|opaque]] surface is either [[Reflection (physics)|reflect]]ed "[[Specular reflection|specularly]]" (that is, in the manner of a mirror), [[scattering|scatter]]ed (that is, reflected with diffuse scattering), or [[absorption (electromagnetic radiation)|absorbed]] – or some combination of these. 
-* Opaque objects that do not reflect specularly (which tend to have rough surfaces) have their color determined by which wavelengths of light they scatter strongly (with the light that is not scattered being absorbed). If objects scatter all wavelengths with roughly equal strength, they appear white. If they absorb all wavelengths, they appear black. 
-* Opaque objects that specularly reflect light of different wavelengths with different efficiencies look like mirrors tinted with colors determined by those differences. An object that reflects some fraction of impinging light and absorbs the rest may look black but also be faintly reflective; examples are black objects coated with layers of enamel or lacquer. 
-* Objects that [[Transparency and translucency|transmit light]] are either ''translucent'' (scattering the transmitted light) or ''transparent'' (not scattering the transmitted light). If they also absorb (or reflect) light of various wavelengths differentially, they appear tinted with a color determined by the nature of that absorption (or that reflectance). 
-* Objects may emit light that they generate from having excited electrons, rather than merely reflecting or transmitting light. The electrons may be excited due to elevated temperature (''[[incandescence]]''), as a result of chemical reactions (''[[chemoluminescence]]''), after absorbing light of other frequencies ("[[fluorescence]]" or "[[phosphorescence]]") or from electrical contacts as in [[light emitting diodes]], or other [[list of light sources|light sources]]. 
- 
-To summarize, the color of an object is a complex result of its surface properties, its transmission properties, and its emission properties, all of which contribute to the mix of wavelengths in the light leaving the surface of the object. The perceived color is then further conditioned by the nature of the ambient illumination, and by the color properties of other objects nearby, and via other characteristics of the perceiving eye and brain. 
== Perception == == Perception ==
- 
-[[File:1Mcolors.png|thumb|This image (when viewed in full size, 1000 pixels wide) contains 1 million pixels, each of a different color. The [[human eye]] can distinguish about 10 million different colors.<ref name="business">{{cite book|first1=Deane B.|last1=Judd|last2=Wyszecki |first2=Günter|title=Color in Business, Science and Industry|publisher=[[Wiley-Interscience]]|series=Wiley Series in Pure and Applied Optics|edition=third|location=New York|year= 1975|page=388|isbn=0-471-45212-2}}</ref>]] 
- 
=== Development of theories of color vision === === Development of theories of color vision ===
:''[[color theory]]'' :''[[color theory]]''
Line 191: Line 37:
==== Tetrachromacy ==== ==== Tetrachromacy ====
-While most humans are ''trichromatic'' (having three types of color receptors), many animals, known as ''[[Tetrachromacy|tetrachromats]]'', have four types. These include some species of [[spider]]s, most [[marsupial]]s, [[bird]]s, [[reptile]]s, and many species of [[fish]]. Other species are sensitive to only two axes of color or do not perceive color at all; these are called ''dichromats'' and ''monochromats'' respectively. A distinction is made between ''retinal tetrachromacy'' (having four pigments in cone cells in the retina, compared to three in trichromats) and ''functional tetrachromacy'' (having the ability to make enhanced color discriminations based on that retinal difference). As many as half of all women are retinal tetrachromats.<ref name="Jameson"/>{{rp|p.256}} The phenomenon arises when an individual receives two slightly different copies of the gene for either the medium- or long-wavelength cones, which are carried on the x-chromosome. To have two different genes, a person must have two x-chromosomes, which is why the phenomenon only occurs in women.<ref name="Jameson"/> For some of these retinal tetrachromats, color discriminations are enhanced, making them functional tetrachromats.<ref name="Jameson">{{cite journal |last1= Jameson |first= K. A. |last2= Highnote |first2= S. M., |last3= Wasserman |first3= L. M.|year = 2001|title = Richer color experience in observers with multiple photopigment opsin genes.|doi = 10.3758/BF03196159|journal = Psychonomic Bulletin and Review|volume = 8|issue = 2|pages = 244–261|url = http://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.3758/BF03196159.pdf|format = PDF|pmid = 11495112}}</ref>+While most humans are ''trichromatic'' (having three types of color receptors), many animals, known as ''[[Tetrachromacy|tetrachromats]]'', have four types. These include some species of [[spider]]s, most [[marsupial]]s, [[bird]]s, [[reptile]]s, and many species of [[fish]]. Other species are sensitive to only two axes of color or do not perceive color at all; these are called ''dichromats'' and ''monochromats'' respectively. A distinction is made between ''retinal tetrachromacy'' (having four pigments in cone cells in the retina, compared to three in trichromats) and ''functional tetrachromacy'' (having the ability to make enhanced color discriminations based on that retinal difference). As many as half of all women are retinal tetrachromats. The phenomenon arises when an individual receives two slightly different copies of the gene for either the medium- or long-wavelength cones, which are carried on the x-chromosome. To have two different genes, a person must have two x-chromosomes, which is why the phenomenon only occurs in women. For some of these retinal tetrachromats, color discriminations are enhanced, making them functional tetrachromats.
==== Synesthesia ==== ==== Synesthesia ====
Line 215: Line 61:
Different colors have been demonstrated to have effects on cognition. For example, researchers at the University of Linz in Austria demonstrated that the color red significantly decreases cognitive functioning in men. Different colors have been demonstrated to have effects on cognition. For example, researchers at the University of Linz in Austria demonstrated that the color red significantly decreases cognitive functioning in men.
== See also == == See also ==
- 
* [[Chromophore]] * [[Chromophore]]
* [[Color analysis (art)]] * [[Color analysis (art)]]
Line 222: Line 67:
* [[Impossible color]] * [[Impossible color]]
* [[International Color Consortium]] * [[International Color Consortium]]
-* [[International Commission on Illumination]]+*[[Linguistic relativity and the color naming debate ]]
* [[Lists of colors]] [[List of colors (compact)|(compact version)]] * [[Lists of colors]] [[List of colors (compact)|(compact version)]]
 +* [[Lüscher color test]]
* [[Neutral color]] * [[Neutral color]]
* [[Pearlescent coating]] including Metal effect pigments * [[Pearlescent coating]] including Metal effect pigments

Revision as of 20:06, 9 March 2019

Blue of the ultramarine variant, similar to the International Klein Blue used by Yves Klein
Enlarge
Blue of the ultramarine variant, similar to the International Klein Blue used by Yves Klein

"There are certain effects of colour that give all men pleasure, and others which jar, almost like a musical discord. A more general development of this sensibility would make possible a new abstract art, an art that should deal with colours as music does with sound." --The Sense of Beauty (1896)

 Monochrome: Illustration: Negroes Fighting in a Tunnel at Night (1882) by Paul Bilhaud, here shown in the 1887 version appropriated by Alphonse Allais as published in Album primo-avrilesque (April fool-ish Album)
Enlarge
Monochrome:
Illustration: Negroes Fighting in a Tunnel at Night (1882) by Paul Bilhaud, here shown in the 1887 version appropriated by Alphonse Allais as published in Album primo-avrilesque (April fool-ish Album)

Related e

Wikipedia
Wiktionary
Shop


Featured:

Colour is the visual perceptual property corresponding in humans to the categories called red, blue, yellow, etc. Color derives from the spectrum of light (distribution of light power versus wavelength) interacting in the eye with the spectral sensitivities of the light receptors. Color categories and physical specifications of color are also associated with objects or materials based on their physical properties such as light absorption, reflection, or emission spectra. By defining a color space colors can be identified numerically by their coordinates.

Because perception of color stems from the varying spectral sensitivity of different types of cone cells in the retina to different parts of the spectrum, colors may be defined and quantified by the degree to which they stimulate these cells. These physical or physiological quantifications of color, however, do not fully explain the psychophysical perception of color appearance.

The science of color is sometimes called chromatics, colorimetry, or simply color science. It includes the perception of color by the human eye and brain, the origin of color in materials, color theory in art, and the physics of electromagnetic radiation in the visible range (that is, what we commonly refer to simply as light).

Contents

Perception

Development of theories of color vision

color theory

Although Aristotle and other ancient scientists had already written on the nature of light and color vision, it was not until Newton that light was identified as the source of the color sensation. In 1810, Goethe published his comprehensive Theory of Colors in which he ascribed physiological effects to color that are now understood as psychological.

In 1801 Thomas Young proposed his trichromatic theory, based on the observation that any color could be matched with a combination of three lights. This theory was later refined by James Clerk Maxwell and Hermann von Helmholtz. As Helmholtz puts it, "the principles of Newton's law of mixture were experimentally confirmed by Maxwell in 1856. Young's theory of color sensations, like so much else that this marvelous investigator achieved in advance of his time, remained unnoticed until Maxwell directed attention to it."

At the same time as Helmholtz, Ewald Hering developed the opponent process theory of color, noting that color blindness and afterimages typically come in opponent pairs (red-green, blue-orange, yellow-violet, and black-white). Ultimately these two theories were synthesized in 1957 by Hurvich and Jameson, who showed that retinal processing corresponds to the trichromatic theory, while processing at the level of the lateral geniculate nucleus corresponds to the opponent theory.

In 1931, an international group of experts known as the Commission internationale de l'éclairage (CIE) developed a mathematical color model, which mapped out the space of observable colors and assigned a set of three numbers to each.

Nonstandard color perception

Color deficiency

If one or more types of a person's color-sensing cones are missing or less responsive than normal to incoming light, that person can distinguish fewer colors and is said to be color deficient or color blind (though this latter term can be misleading; almost all color deficient individuals can distinguish at least some colors). Some kinds of color deficiency are caused by anomalies in the number or nature of cones in the retina. Others (like central or cortical achromatopsia) are caused by neural anomalies in those parts of the brain where visual processing takes place.

Tetrachromacy

While most humans are trichromatic (having three types of color receptors), many animals, known as tetrachromats, have four types. These include some species of spiders, most marsupials, birds, reptiles, and many species of fish. Other species are sensitive to only two axes of color or do not perceive color at all; these are called dichromats and monochromats respectively. A distinction is made between retinal tetrachromacy (having four pigments in cone cells in the retina, compared to three in trichromats) and functional tetrachromacy (having the ability to make enhanced color discriminations based on that retinal difference). As many as half of all women are retinal tetrachromats. The phenomenon arises when an individual receives two slightly different copies of the gene for either the medium- or long-wavelength cones, which are carried on the x-chromosome. To have two different genes, a person must have two x-chromosomes, which is why the phenomenon only occurs in women. For some of these retinal tetrachromats, color discriminations are enhanced, making them functional tetrachromats.

Synesthesia

In certain forms of synesthesia/ideasthesia, perceiving letters and numbers (grapheme–color synesthesia) or hearing musical sounds (music–color synesthesia) will lead to the unusual additional experiences of seeing colors. Behavioral and functional neuroimaging experiments have demonstrated that these color experiences lead to changes in behavioral tasks and lead to increased activation of brain regions involved in color perception, thus demonstrating their reality, and similarity to real color percepts, albeit evoked through a non-standard route.

Afterimages

After exposure to strong light in their sensitivity range, photoreceptors of a given type become desensitized. For a few seconds after the light ceases, they will continue to signal less strongly than they otherwise would. Colors observed during that period will appear to lack the color component detected by the desensitized photoreceptors. This effect is responsible for the phenomenon of afterimages, in which the eye may continue to see a bright figure after looking away from it, but in a complementary color.

Afterimage effects have also been utilized by artists, including Vincent van Gogh.

Color naming

Colors vary in several different ways, including hue (shades of red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet), saturation, brightness, and gloss. Some color words are derived from the name of an object of that color, such as "orange" or "salmon", while others are abstract, like "red".

In the 1969 study Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution, Brent Berlin and Paul Kay describe a pattern in naming "basic" colors (like "red" but not "red-orange" or "dark red" or "blood red", which are "shades" of red). All languages that have two "basic" color names distinguish dark/cool colors from bright/warm colors. The next colors to be distinguished are usually red and then yellow or green. All languages with six "basic" colors include black, white, red, green, blue, and yellow. The pattern holds up to a set of twelve: black, gray, white, pink, red, orange, yellow, green, blue, purple, brown, and azure (distinct from blue in Russian and Italian, but not English).

Associations

Individual colors have a variety of cultural associations such as national colors (in general described in individual color articles and color symbolism). The field of color psychology attempts to identify the effects of color on human emotion and activity. Chromotherapy is a form of alternative medicine attributed to various Eastern traditions. Colors have different associations in different countries and cultures.

Different colors have been demonstrated to have effects on cognition. For example, researchers at the University of Linz in Austria demonstrated that the color red significantly decreases cognitive functioning in men.

See also




Unless indicated otherwise, the text in this article is either based on Wikipedia article "Color" or another language Wikipedia page thereof used under the terms of the GNU Free Documentation License; or on research by Jahsonic and friends. See Art and Popular Culture's copyright notice.

Personal tools